In this chapter, we are concerned with how people
process sentences. How are words meaning integrated with the structural
meanings of sentence. We cannot derive the meaning, or meanings of a sentence,
from the meanings of the individual words alone. WORDS ORDER is one aspect
crucial in many languages, including English.
FORMAL GRAMMARS
We describe several of these formal linguistic
grammars for several reasons. First, they have provided a major impetus for
psychological research on language, particularly during the past ten years,
when psychologists used them as tentative models of how sentences are
processed. Further and more important for our purposes, each type of formal
grammar reflects one or another important aspect of the structure of language.
(( The pterodactyl) (entertains (the lonesome
child))).
This bracketing of the sentences can be represented
by a labeled tree diagram, shown in Figure 4.1, that reveals the hierarchical
structure more clearly. The DETERMINER (Det) the and NOUN (N) pterodactyl combine
to form a NOUN PHRASE (NP), which is the subject of the SENTENCE (S). the
predicate, or VERB PHRASE (VP), is composed of a TRANSITIVE VERB (Vt),
entertains, and another noun phrase.
The second NP is the direct object and is composed of a determiner the and ADJECTIVE (Adj) lonesome, and noun, child.
Phrase-Structure Grammars are among the simplest of
the formal grammars. They describe a structural analysis of a sentence in order
that the words actually occur. For this reason, they are also called
SURFACE-STRUCTURED ANALYSES.
“Semantic” Grammar and Artificial Intelligence, these
grammars have been exclusively syntactic in that the meanings of words and sentences
are not formally considered. A number of linguists have recognized this abd
have proposed alternatives to the purely syntactic approach of transformational
grammars. To take one example, Fillmore (1968) proposed a CASE GRAMMAR, which
treats the verb of asentence as the central organizing unit. One advantage of
this is analogous to the realtion between transformational grammars and
phrase-structured grammars.
Competence
and Performance, in the sense that any adequate grammar captures what people
“know” about a language, the grammar describes LINGUISTIC COMPETENCE. If the
rules of grammar can be used to describe formally all the grammatical sentences
of alanguage and no ungrammatical ones, and if people can do the same thing
intuitively, then in the sense the competence of the grammar is equivalent to
the linguistic competence of the people who speak the language. But it does not
necessarily specify how people go about producing the output, or LINGUISTIC
PERFORMANCE.
SENTENCE PERCEPTION
We usually begin to process a part of the sentences
before the rest comes in, and this processing involves several kinds of
operations. We assign meanings to words; we analyze syntactic and semantic
relations; and we try to integrate information as quickly as possible so as to
minimize the load on immediate memory.
Segmentation, we can crudely partition our memory
system into two general components, WORKING MEMORY and LONG-TERM MEMORY. Our
long-term memory comprises all of our knowledge, including episodic and
semantic memory. If the interpretation of sentence processing is correct, then
we would expect people to forget a sentence segment that has just been
processed. On this view, when a sentence is heard, the words are held in immediate
memory until an interpretable segment is completed, then while the next
sequence of words comes in, the first segment is processed.
Sentence organization, one way to reveal the
organization of such sentences is to give someone a word from a memorized sentence
and ask for the first other word from that comes to mind. Whatever the
organizational pattern of a sentence is, it is not a left-to right sequence,
and the units are not only individual words.
Processing Ambiguous Sentences, sentences may be
ambiguous in one of two ways, lexical or syntactic. LEXICAL AMBIGUITY occurs
when a word may be interpreted in two different ways, as in sentences below:
1.a The sailors liked the port from Portugal
1.b The sailor liked the port in Portugal
In
sentence (1.a), the more likely interpretation would be the red wine, whereas
in sentence (1.b), the more likely interpretation would be the harbor.
SYNTATIC
AMBIGUITY occurs when a sentence can be organized in two different ways, as in
sentences below:
2.a They (are visiting) relatives in Chicago
2.b
They are from Chicago
In
the first example, the word they does
not refer to the relatives, and the word visiting
function as a verb. In the second, they
does refer to relatives, and visiting is used as a modifier of relatives.
SENTENCE
COMPREHENSION
In general, we consider that a sentence has been
understood or comprehended when we are able to use the information derived from
the sentence in some appropriate way. One way to asses sentence comprehension
was developed by Lee MCMahon (1963), using an ingenious adaptation of the
successive matching paradigm.
3.a The car hit the truck
3.b The truck was hit by the car. Which
sentence takes longer to comprehend ?
In some early versions of Chomky’s
transformational-generative grammar, passive sentences were considered to be
more complex than active sentences, and so it was expected that sentence (25b)
would be psychologically more complex and thus take longer to process. This
expectation was confirmed in a number of studies motivated by
transformational-generative grammars (McMahon, 1963; Gough, 1965, 1966; Slobin,
1966). Each of these experiments used a variant of McMahon’s successive
matching task, and they found that passive sentences took longer to verify than
did active sentences. Since linguistic theory no longer provided a rationale
for the relative difficulty of active and passive sentences, other reasons for
their relative difficulty had to be uncovered.
MEMORY FOR SENTENCES
We have already seen that sentences can be
remembered in two general ways-verbatim or in some interpreted format. VERBATIM
MEMORY for sentences corresponds to memory for surface structure, the exact
words in the order that they had appeared in. the INTERPRETED FORMAT refers to
our memory for a sentence that no longer has the exact words in their original
order, and this corresponds to a generalized “deep-structured” format.
Linguistic Deep Structures, the grammar
characterized any given sentence in terms of a surface structure that is
derivable from an underlying kernel or deep structure. To understand a sentence
it is necessary (though not of course sufficient) to reconstruct its representation
on each level, including the transformational level where the kernel sentences
underlying a given sentence can be though of, in a sense, as the ‘elementary
content elements’ out of which this sentence is constructed. As syntactic
theory developed, it became evident that one had a wide choice of “deep
structures” even within transformational-generative grammar, one had a choice
of Chomsky’s 1957 model, in which active sentence were considered to be
“simpler” than passive sentences, and his 1965 model, in which active and
passive sentences were equally “complex.”
Imagery, if the words refer to concrete, easily
visualized things, they are easier to remember than words that refer to
abstract concepts. If people are asked to visualize the referents of the words,
they remember them better than when they are not asked to do so. These, as well
as many other findings of the effectiveness of visual imagery for memory,
suggest that the information expressed by sentences might also be represented
in memory in the form of images.
4.a The
gloves were made by tailors.
4.b The
gloves were made by hand
If only the verbatim record of the sentences had
been retained, then tailors and hand should be equally effective as
prompts for remembering the sentences. Tailors
was a more effective prompt for later recall, presumably because
information about how things are made is not as central to the meaning of a
sentence as who made things (the agent of a sentence).
Abstract Conceptual Representation, when imagery is
not appropriate, we use other devices to comprehend and remember the. We can understand
and sentences like
5. Philosophers
are concerned with epistemological issues.
With sentences like this one, and with high-imagery
sentences as well, we remember the gist and often forget the specific words and
sentences themselves.
6. He
sent a letter bout it to Galileo, the great scientist.
The italicized sentence above is the critical
sentence, although people did not know it while hearing the story. After zero,
80, 160 syllables, the story was interrupted and a test sentence was presented.
People were to espond “yes” if that test sentence had occurred verbatim, and
“no” if it differed in any way from the critical sentence. A test sentence
could be any one of these four types:
IDENTICAL:
He sent a letter about it to Galileo, the great Italian scientist. (no change)
SEMANTIC
: Galileo, the great Italian scientist, sent him a letter about it. (change in
word order and meaning )
PASSIVE
: A letter about it was sent to Galileo, the great Italian scientist. (no
change in meaning, change in sentence voice )
FORMAL
: He sent Galileo, the great Italian scientist, a letter about it. ( no change
in meaning, change in order of phrases )
Sentences
Comprising a Complex Idea
FOUR : (t) The ants in the kitchen ate the sweet
jelly which was on the table.
THREEs
: (a) The ants ate the sweet jelly which was on the table. (a) The ants in the kitchen ate the jelly
which was on the table.
(t) The ants in the kitchen ate the sweet
jelly.
TWOs
: (a) The ants in the kitchen ate the jelly.
(a&t) The ants ate the sweet jelly.
(t) The sweet jelly was on the table.
(t) The ants ate the jelly which was on the
table.
ONEs
: (a) The ants were in the kitchen.
(a) The jelly was on the table.
(t) The jelly was sweet.
(t) The ants ate the jelly.
other Fours : (t) The warm breeze
blowing from the sea stirred the heavy
evening air.
(t) The rock which rolled down the mountain
crushed the tiny hut at the edge of the woods.
(t) The old man resting on the couch read
the story in the newspaper.
COPREHENSION AND KNOWLEDGE OF THE
WORLD
Comprehension,
broadly conceived, is an active process that uses prior knowledge to interpret
and organize the meanings of utterances.
(ex) John remembered to let the dog out.
We
know that the dog is supposed to be out. However, there is a qualitative
difference between these two items of information. That the dog is out can be
logically inferred from sentence (ex), and is, therefore called an INFERENCE.
The latter information, that the dog is supposed to be out, is a
PRESUPPOSITION.
When
comprehending the sentence, we assume that the presupposition is true, even
though it is not necessarily true.
Neither sentences nor longer
utterances are comprehended in isolation. If a semantically rich grammar is
necessary for dealing with sentences, an equally rich “grammar” would be needed
with for dealing with discourse in general.
No comments:
Post a Comment